How We Hear (May 1977)

Home | Audio Magazine | Stereo Review magazine | Good Sound | Troubleshooting



The audio enthusiast, like nearly every other human being, takes most, if not all, his physiological and psychological processes for granted. Indeed, most of us are typically unaware of those internal events which enable us to live our lives and experience our environment. Only now and then tingling fingers, ringing ears, a grumbling stomach or painful joints remind us indirectly of the specific sensory systems which gather the sensations and information the brain requires.

Of our 10 or possibly 11 individual sensory systems (each of which has unique, specialized receptors that respond to different kinds of stimuli), we tend to think of vision first. Indeed, since the great majority of research into sensory mechanisms has concerned itself with vision, we have more information about the visual sense modality than about any of the others. Nevertheless, hearing is extremely important in the lives of humans and not merely because we enjoy music. It has been long observed, for example, that deafness typically has a more disastrous effect on the psychological development of an individual than does blindness; not being able to communicate aurally, vocally and by the uttered word isolates us in a way blindness does not.

An audiophile, of course, has a strong interest in the adequate functioning of his or her auditory system. Knowledge about it, while of intrinsic interest, can also enhance many listening experiences, as well as provide a broader basis for the rational selection and evaluation of various stereo components. Information about auditory mechanisms also will enable one to more adequately care for this vital and largely irreplacable link in the high-fidelity chain. We will first consider the auditory system from an anatomical basis, then we will discuss several physiological mechanisms involved in the processes of hearing. To more fully appreciate the anatomy and manner of functioning of our hearing system, let's first briefly discuss the biological origins and embryological development of the auditory system.

Origin and Development

In the evolutionary or phylogenetic sense, the auditory system is a derivative of a receptor system originally concerned with balance and equilibrium, not hearing. This sense modality, which we now call the vestibular sense, responds to rotation, our orientation in space, balance, acceleration, deceleration, and similar forces acting on the body. Wild amusement park rides strongly stimulate the receptors for this sense, which are located in the three-semi-circular canals shown in Figs. 1 and 2. Vertigo and car-, air- and boat-sickness are all products of vestibular system functioning. Among animals with backbones, the vestibular systems of fishes, amphibia (frogs, salamanders), reptiles (snakes, turtles), birds and mammals are remarkably similar and differ mostly in details and the degree of refinement. The similarity between the fishes' vestibular apparatus and our own is apparent in Figs. 1 and 2.


Fig. 1 --Vestibular apparatus of a fish.

The vestibular system of the fish derives from the same group of cells that form the fishes' lateral line "organ."

This lateral line system of receptors is readily seen on tropical fish and on many edible varieties at the grocers as a thin line midway down the side of the fish, which extends from just behind the gillplate to the base of the fishes' tail. The receptors of this lateral line system of fish are tiny hair cells, called neuromasts. These hair cells are themselves very similar to and appear to function much like the hair cells of our inner ear which are the actual receptors for hearing. References to Fig. 4 will give some idea of these hair cells in the human inner ear. Thus, the ultimate origin of our hearing receptors appears to have been a sensory system in the fish, the lateral line system, with its many hair cells situated in a lengthwise line down the fishes' side. Our auditory system appears to be a modification of the vestibular apparatus to accommodate the hair cells of the lateral line system.

Not surprisingly, in view of their relationship, the vestibular and auditory apparatus in the human develop nearly on a par with one another during gestation. Of the 12 cranial nerves which directly enter the lower part of the brain, the eighth cranial nerve is concerned with both hearing and equilibrium. We call this eighth cranial nerve the statoacoustic nerve to acknowledge its important equilibrium, vestibular and auditory functions. This nerve carries neural impulses from the vestibular receptors of the inner ear to the appropriate regions in the brain many months before the infant is born. In fact, study of human fetuses indicates that the vestibular receptors respond to stimuli and the statoacoustic nerve transmits neural impulses generated by these receptors to the brain by the 12th week of gestation.


Fig. 2--Left vestibular apparatus and cochlea of human being. Letters identifying the following: a.) semi-circular canal; b.) cochlea; c.) organ of Corti.

The auditory portions of this system develop more slowly. After 24 to 25 weeks of growth, the human fetus appears to have an auditory system that is complete in most details. If not complete, it is, nevertheless, functional at this point in time. Analyses have shown, for example, that the varied auditory and vestibular stimuli a fetus receives encourage the growth of the neural centers which receive these impulses from the statoacoustic nerve. A fetus will typically experience extensive vestibular stimulation, depending on the mobility of its mother. The range of auditory experience is restricted, but not eliminated by the niotic fluid which encompasses the fetus and fills its external auditory meatuses (air passages to the ear drum). The vestibular and auditory stimuli that impinge on the fetus and to which it can respond before birth contribute greatly to the development of both its vestibular and auditory sensory systems. At birth, then, these sensory systems are totally or highly matured. The receptors and neural transmission "lines" are complete and sufficiently well-developed to begin functioning in the outside world at a comparatively sophisticated level. Numerous studies have shown, for example, that newborn infants, less than two hours old, respond to a variety of sounds. More recently it has been shown that an infant is capable of discriminating familiar from unfamiliar voices within days after birth.

What are the anatomical characteristics of the auditory system? How do the many parts relate to produce auditory sensitivity? In the next section we shall consider the physical attributes of our hearing mechanism and the manner in which the vibrations in air eventually become the auditory experiences that so enrich our lives.

General Anatomy

Invariably when we think of the ear and of hearing, what first comes to mind is the accessory structure attached to each side of our heads, the cartilaginous pinna or auricle which aids in selecting and focusing vibrations at our ear drum. Some may think also of the tiny bones of the middle ear which transmit the vibrations of the eardrum to more internally situated structures. However, even these bones, called ossicles, are accessories-items convenient for but not essential to hearing in vertebrates. In a sense, the pinna and ossicles perform a preamp function-equalization and first-stage amplification. The real "ear" is the internal or inner ear, a coiled structure situated in an appropriately coiled canal within the temporal bone of the head. This coiled structure, called the cochlea, is shown in Fig. 2. In humans, the cochlea and its canal are coiled 2 3/4 times. The auditory sense receptors, the microscopic hair cells of Fig. 4, are situated here within part of the cochlea from one end of the spiral to the other.

Thus, the pinna, auditory canal, eardrum, and ossicles enhance our ability to hear, but they are not the auditory receptors. To be sure, rupture of the eardrum and disruption of the three ossicles would result in a catastrophe from the audio enthusiast's point of view-a hearing deficit of from 30- to 50 decibels magnitude, but not complete deafness.


Fig. 3--Diagram of mammalian auditory apparatus. Letters identify the following: a.) auditory meatus; b.) eardrum; c.) ossicles; d.) oval window; e.) round window; f.) uncoiled cochlea; g.) basilar membrane; h.) tympanic chamber; i.) vestibular chamber; j.) middle chamber; k.) eustachian tube.

Outer and Middle Ear

The relatively simple anatomy of the outer and middle ear doesn't show the complexity of problems their evolutionary development has solved. Reference to Fig. 3 will make clear the fact that the pinna and external auditory meatus are a funnel.

The human meatus is about one inch long with a natural vibration frequency of just less than 4000 Hz. The large amplitude sound pressure waves in air are funneled to the eardrum or tympanic membrane. The eardrum, remarkably, manages to reflect back very little incident energy and to transmit most of it. Frequencies up to 2000 Hz cause the eardrum to vibrate as a whole, like a disc, with maximum oscillation at the bottom of the drum.

As the frequency increases, the mode of vibration becomes increasingly complex with the drumhead no longer vibrating as a whole. The three ossicles, attached to flexible membranes at both input and output, transform the high amplitude, low pressure input movements of the eardrum into low amplitude, high pressure movements at the output point of the ossicles, the footplate of the stirrup (see Fig. 3); they function as a transformer.

Pressure at the eardrum (where it begins) is approximately five percent of the output pressure transmitted from the stirrup footplate to the oval window of the cochlea, the other flexible membrane (Fig. 3). As you might imagine, such a mechanical advantage could cause damage. To avert this, the two smallest striated muscles in the entire body are attached to two ossicles, respectively. These two muscles protect the hair cells from most overly loud noises and the huge osscilations derived from them by increasing the internal friction of the system, thus reducing the ease of sound transmission. However, some intense noises (usually man-made) may have such an abrupt wavefront as to reach the hair cells unattenuated because these small muscles require between 60-150 milliseconds to contract. Permanent damage to hair cells is the result in this case; they are simply sheared off. The behavioral effect is a frequency region where one does not hear any sounds at all. Fortunately, most sounds are frequency composites or many of us would have more difficulty than we do.

The fantastic sensitivity of the ear can be attributed to nature's use of several transformers--each of which largely ameliorates the energy losses at the several "interfaces" in the auditory receptive system. The first transformer between the air and the cochlear fluid is an hydraulic piston transformer. It includes the eardrum, the ossicles, the oval window, and the inner ear fluid. Calculations by Dr. Georg von Bekesy (1961 Nobel Prize winner for his audition research) indicate that the middle ear anatomy does, in fact, minimize energy loss in passing vibrations to the cochlea. Measurement in a variety of mammals has shown, also, that absolute auditory thresholds (minimal required pressure sensitivity to produce a neuro-electric response) are nearly equal.

That is, little more energy can be conserved for transmission by further modifications to this already highly efficient system. This highly evolved capacity to transmit rather than to reflect incident energy is the special attribute of the outer and middle ear.

The very small inertia of the ossicles provides one basis for this capacity.

Of course, high signal sensitivity implies high sensitivity to noise as well.

Fortunately, the ear does establish and maintain an adequate signal-to-noise ratio. The effects of the many internal sources of noise (e.g. vocalization, chewing, breathing, muscular contraction, circulation of blood, and neural discharge) appear to have been minimized by judicious placement of sensitive components. External sources of noises must, of course, be handled differently. It is known presently, for example, that the S/N ratio is improved at several levels of the auditory nervous chain. A primary mechanism involved here is the inhibition of that nervous discharge which is largely random (as opposed to periodic) as is the frequency of most noise, a common mode rejection of sorts.


Fig. 4--Cross section through human cochlea. Letters identify: a.) vestibular chamber; b.) middle chamber; c.) tympanic membrane; g.) nerve fibers.

Once the energy has reached the inner ear, a second transformer comes into play. This transformer utilizes vertical pressure on a flexible membrane, the basilar membrane, to create a longitudinal stress in a second, inflexible membrane, the tectorial membrane (Fig. 4). The actual receptors, the hair cells, being situated somewhat obliquely between these two membranes, as shown, have their hairs bent over at the point where they contact the stiff, tectorial membrane by the shearing force thus generated. This second transformer has been called a shearing transformer.

Let us look more closely at the elements of this mechanism and their interrelations.

About 12,000 hair cells (that's all) are situated within the spiral cochlea (Figs. 2 and 3). The cochlea is divided by two membranes into three chambers, two of which communicate with one another at the tip of the cochlea, through the helicotrema. These two chambers, the "upper" or scala tympani and the "lower" or scala vestibule, are filled with a fluid, perilymph (Fig. 3), which is nearly identical to the watery fluid that baths the central nervous system, called cerebrospinal fluid. The third, non-communicating chamber, often called the cochlear duct, is between the other two chambers. Fluid fills this chamber also. This fluid, termed endolymph, is more viscous than the perilymph of the two other connecting chambers. The ossicular vibrations are transmitted to the fluid of the upper- and lower-connecting chambers by the stirrup which rests against another membrane, the oval window. Since these chambers are completely filled with watery perilymph fluid, the fluid pushes out on all the boundary surfaces. One of these surfaces is the flexible, basilar membrane. The basilar membrane is so termed because it forms part of the floor of the cochlear duct, thereby differentiating the middle and lower chambers. This membrane, which von Bekesy discovered is under no tension whatever, is displaced up and down by the forces exerted on it from beneath. As mentioned earlier, this displacement ultimately causes a shearing force to be exerted on the hair cell receptors which are situated in the cochlear duct between the basilar membrane and the rigid tectorial membrane above them. As can be seen from Figs. 2 and 4, this "receptor structure" occupies only a small part of the middle cochlear chamber. Endolymph fills the rest performing a vibration-isolating function. The basilar membrane, tectorial membrane, and receptor cells in between, which form the receptor structure, are usually called the organ of Corti. All this may appear to be an unusually complex system to transfer vibrations to cells which can respond to them, but in fact, it's elegant simplicity becomes increasingly apparent as one learns more about it.

By comparison with the processes involved merely in pitch discrimination, things are simple indeed. A concluding fact or two may provide an adequate perspective.

The human eardrum has an area of about 70 sq. mm. The area of the oval window to which vibrations are finally transmitted is only about 3 sq. mm.

Despite this small area, the approximate minimal energy level of a 1000 Hz tone that we can sense is 0.0002 dyn/cm2 (dyne=energy to accelerate 1 gm at 1 cm/sect). Were our hearing any more sensitive, we would be distracted continuously by molecular bombardment of our eardrums. In a manner of speaking, then, we could hear differences in temperature.

The prodigious accomplishment already described is, however, only a fraction of the story. For we can discriminate identical and different tones of subtly different sound intensities, perhaps 250 distinct intensity levels. We are able to make numerous pitch discriminations-about 1,000 of them within the range of 20 to 16,000 Hz. Moreover, we are sensitive to the "mass" or "volume" of sound as distinct from loudness; and finally, some of us can make pitch discriminations of such accuracy that these persons have been said to have perfect (!) pitch. These phonomena, observed and studied in the psychophysics of audition, deserve our attention. Yet, we have one more step to consider first-the generation of neural impulses and transmission of these to the appropriate regions of the brain.

The Inner Ear

Properly speaking, the hair cells are transducers, not transformers. Up until this point in the sequence, the energy has been mechanical. Once the hair cells respond, however, we are no longer dealing with mechanical energy. The hair cells have generated an electrical potential which neurally represents the mechanical energy transmitted to them. This potential, often called a generator potential, is one of several potentials that can be recorded from the inner ear. The actual mechanisms for the transduction process are not yet known. Despite that, our knowledge concerning auditory mechanisms has been advanced, and we can profitably consider the cochlear mechanisms for loudness discrimination and pitch discrimination. The organ of Corti concerns itself with other auditory dimensions in addition to these. Perusal of these two processes will demonstrate the ingenious evolutionary solutions that enable us to perceive extraordinarily subtle auditory differences in loudness, pitch and a combination of the two.

The Organ of Corti

The greater the vibratory motion of the basilar membrane, the more hair cells it stimulates to "fire." The more hair cells that fire, the louder will be the perceived sound. As is often the case, this straight-forward relation oversimplifies the actual state of affairs. Suffice it to say that the outermost column of hair cells (which are arrayed in four columns as seen in Fig. 4) is more receptive to vibration amplitude differences than any of the other columns. They derive this enhanced sensitivity to amplitude from their physical position and unique neural connections. These hair cells appear to provide us with most of our loudness information. Obviously, the loudness information is in addition to though not necessarily independent of pitch information. In fact, as many know, these two dimensions of auditory experience are closely interrelated.

Pitch discrimination is partly a static and partly a dynamic process. The untensioned basilar membrane is wider at the apex of the cochlear coil than it is at the base where the coil originates. From Fig. 2, we might expect the opposite, that the basilar membrane narrows as the coil it's in narrows. Since the mass of the basilar membrane is least at the base (where it's narrowest), it's natural vibration frequency is higher there and it is most sensitive to high frequency vibrations in that region. Conversely, the greater mass of the basilar membrane at the apex of the cochlear coil renders that area more sensitive to vibrations of low frequency. Clearly the anatomical shape of the basilar membrane predisposes it to vibrate maximally at different regions along its length because, as it goes from base to apex, it gets wider and wider. Additionally, the higher the frequency, the more localized the area of vibration; very low frequencies (below 50 Hz) induce the entire membrane to vibrate as a whole. The waves set up in the basilar membrane are not standing waves from one side of the membrane to the other (piano string vibration), as had been theorized a century ago by Hermann von Helmholtz. Dr. von Bekesy has shown that, because the basilar membrane is not under tension, it vibrates similar to a rope, tied at one end and waved up and down at the other. This kind of vibratory motion is a travelling wave. The envelope of a travelling wave will show maximum vibration amplitude at various regions along an unstretched membrane as a function of the frequency of vibration and the physical qualities of the membrane. This characteristic is shown in Fig. 5. While the upper limit of this "tonotopic" organization and the frequency discrimination deriving from it is not known, it is clear that this static mechanism contributes greatly to our abilities to discriminate pitch.


Fig. 5--Travelling waves of different frequencies on the basilar membrane (after von Bekesy).

The second pitch discrimination mechanism that operates in the organ of Corti is a dynamic, neural one.

In recent experiments, electrodes about a millionth of a meter in diameter (microelectrodes) have been placed in individual cells of the auditory nerve or at other auditory central nervous system locations. Research with these "microelectrodes" has shown that special nerve fibers to (not from) the organ of Corti inhibit the response of hair cells immediately above and below the region of maximum basilar membrane movement.

That is, these special fibers make it more difficult for hair cells situated to either side of the maximally stimulated hair cells to respond. The effect is to increase or "sharpen" the discriminability of one frequency from another. Of additional importance here is the anatomical fact that all inner hair cells (Fig. 4), which are known to be primarily involved in pitch discrimination, are connected to their own individual neural fiber going to the central nervous system. Thus, precise, detailed information regarding pitch is transmitted to the higher neural centers.

Not surprisingly, much more happens to the neural signals that traverse these 25,000 or so fibers once they enter the central nervous system, for elegant though the preceding mechanisms are, they are not sufficient to account for the incredible pitch discrimination many people show. One of the mechanisms which occurs inside the central nervous system that is known to increase our pitch discrimination is the following.

Neuro-physiological research with microelectrodes has established that frequency discrimination takes place at various levels of the neural circuitry as the impulses proceed to the portions of the brain that ultimately process and analyze the auditory impulses. Inside the brain, but not at the highest (cortical) level, neural inhibitory mechanisms appear to reduce further the neural response to random (aperiodic) input and to further accentuate a specific response. All this occurs in a system which ordinarily does not "follow" or keep track with stimulus frequencies beyond 200 Hz.

This account is necessarily brief.

Nevertheless, it is hoped that some idea of auditory system origin, development, and functioning has been derived from it. The elegance of this system is surely obvious. What should also be apparent is that a great deal of signal processing has taken place even before the signal reaches its ultimate destination, the auditory regions of the cortex of the brain. A discussion of the processes occurring in the several brain areas concerned with audition would provide further insight into the psychology of audition. That enterprise, connecting the physiology and psychology of hearing, transcends the present topic in complexity and requires a separate treatment.

References

1. Green, D.M. Audition. Annual Review of Psychology, 20: 105-128, 1969.

2. Milner, P.M. Physiological Psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1970.

3. Minkowski, A. (ed.) Regional Development of the Brain in Early Life. Philadelphia, 1967.

4. Ochs, S. Elements of Neurophysiology. New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1965.

5. Peele, T.L. Neuroanatomic Basis for Clinical Neurology. New York: J. Wiley and Sons, 1957.

6. Peiper, A. Cerebral Function in Infancy and Childhood. New York: Consultants Bureau, 1963.

7. Romer, A.S. The Vertebrate Body. New York: W.B. Saunders, 1956.

8. Stevens, S.S. (ed.) Handbook of Experimental Psychology, New York: Wiley and Sons, 1957.

9. von Bekesy, G. Traveling waves as frequency analyzers in the cochlea. Nature, 225; 1207-1209, 1970.

10. Zemlin W.R. Speech and Hearing Science. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, 1968.

(Source: Audio magazine, May 1977; Dr. A. Joseph Ray, Jr., Ph.D.* [Professor of Psychology, East Stroudsburg State College, East Stroudsburg, Penna. 18301, USA])

= = = =

Prev. | Next

Top of Page    Home

Updated: Friday, 2017-01-13 17:18 PST