.
“What’s wrong with conventional listening evaluations?”
Glad you asked. Although the performance of a preamp, line stage, or power
amp in your system is of primary interest (at least for now), there are so
many j variables that such evaluation is difficult, uncertain, and likely to
change. These variables include:
1. The rest of your audio system. You might like a low damping factor tube
amp because its speaker Z-dependent bass peak compensates a speaker/room bass
attenuation. Or you might like your preamp's HF rolloff because it "tames" the
HF edginess and grain mess of your CDs.
2. Recordings. Most popular music, and even some classical recordings, have
been "produced" with bass rolloff, compression, and various midrange/treble "equalization." This
is because of the infamous "loudness wars"; sales are proportional
to dB SPL.
3. LF hearing loss at the usual lower than-live playback SPL (Fletcher Munson
effect). Together with van able #2, this is a good reason to use (good) tone
controls, instead of colored components (expensive tone controls).
4. The uncertainties in evaluating a component. Preconceived notions about
what a different or modified component "should" sound like, listening
from a different position (moving your head 1" can make over 1dB difference
at 10kHz), and relying on memory are notorious in their ability to influence
perception.
5. Not having heard live music (especially unamplified acoustic instruments
and singers) for too long. How many times have you tweaked your system and
been satisfied with it for some time, and then went to a con cert and thought, "so
this is what it should sound like"?
If, on the other hand, the component in question has the highest possible
intrinsic fidelity to its input signal, it might not currently provide the
best overall system sound. But when you up grade other components and/or get
better recordings, or upgrade from CDs to SACDs, DVD-As, or vinyl, that component's
superior transparency will become evident.
INTRINSIC FIDELITY TEST
Figure 1 shows the general setup. The input/output levels should be matched
to 0.05dB, because wideband differences of 0.2dB are audible. If the component
has significant frequency response variations, the best you can do is level
match at 1kHz, or some frequency at which the component's gain is around the
300Hz 3kHz average.
A power amp should be loaded with a reactive network approximating real world
speaker loads. An actual speaker would be best, but it would need to be acoustically
isolated (e.g., wrapped in blankets in a large box in another room).
Preamps can be load-sensitive, so if the attenuator and listening amp don't
pro vide a low enough load impedance, add a parallel load resistor. The selection
of attenuator pot value can help.
The component should be driven to several dB below clipping if a power amp,
or to a representative normal-use level if a preamp. The component might need
input attenuation or gain. Phono preamps can be tested if preceded by a very
accurate inverse RIAA network, with the “In” position of the A/B switch coming
from the inverse RIAA net work’s input.
To ensure near (but not at) clipping level drive for a power amp, an oscilloscope
at the output is best. But as a substitute, you can set the power amp’s gain
(with its own input attenuator if it has one, or an external one) if you know
the source player’s peak output. With CD players, the Red Guide full-scale
output standard (0 dBFS) is 2.00V RMS sine (±2.83Vpeak).The CD and SACD players
I’ve measured (Sony) produce up to ±3.2V peak with “hot” recordings.
LISTENING
In this test, any audible difference is undesirable. If the output sounds
better than the input, you have the “expensive tone control” effect. (Or, as
some ads would have you believe, some magic time-machine circuit that removes recording studio distortion!)
With a very transparent, neutral, and quiet component, you might not hear
any difference. If this is so, congratulations! Following are some suggestions
for maximizing the sensitivity of the test, while also guarding against the
influence of thought processes (when listening for very subtle effects, thoughts,
expectations, and so on can make you believe you hear something that disappears
with enough patience, relaxation, and persistent repetition):
Above: Fig. 1: Intrinsic fidelity test.
1. Use high-quality headphones, which can remove room acoustics, eliminate
frequency response changes due to head movements, and have much higher resolution
than most speakers. It's good to also listen with your speakers, because a
very slight effect heard on high-quality phones could be inaudible in normal
speaker listening. And I suppose it's possible that the room-enhanced speaker
stereo ambience sound field could be more revealing of subtle spatial effects
that the tested component could be producing.
2. Choose the best available recordings.
Vinyl, SACD, and the rare DVD-A that uses its full resolution format are the
most revealing. Conventional 16/44 CDs just don't have as much resolution and
grain-free transparency. However, because in this test it's only differences,
not preferences, that we're listening for, even pink noise and electronic impulses
can reveal some effects. But the best musical sources will, of course, be the
most revealing.
3. Be prepared to spend at least an hour with the AJB switch. I recommend
several switching methods:
a) listen to an entire musical piece in each switch position
b) switch after
the first few seconds of a piece (preferably one that "starts with a bang"),
then quickly reset the track to the beginning.
Repeat at least 20 times.
c) Find a sustained violin ensemble harmony, piano chord, sung note, horn
tone, reverb tail, and so on, and rapidly switch back and forth.
Listen without thinking about which position the switch is in.
It's best to allow all thoughts to evaporate and just enjoy the music.
If you notice a difference, that's when it's time to focus. But remember
that focusing on one thing excludes others, so after analyzing one perception, "remember
to forget" again, and proceed.
4. The following might seem silly, but it can be revealing: temporarily connect
both switch positions to the same point (input or output). Then perfect your
acting ability by pretending you're still comparing two different signals.
You might be surprised at the influence of the normal music variations, or
of head motions if using speakers. If x else, this silly exercise can provide
a "noise floor calibration" for establishing a limit on reliable
and consistent audibility thresholds.
THE EFFECT OF SMALL FREQUENCY RESPONSE VARIATIONS
As I mentioned, 0.2dB variations can be audible. However, they're not necessarily
perceived as such. Through a phenomenon that can be called "psychoacoustic
mapping," frequency response variations can affect imaging, focus, and
spaciousness. Generally, bass/lower midrange emphasis increases perception
of space, and treble emphasis increases perception of tonal details and image
focus.
A little experimentation with an equalizer or tone controls will help you
recognize these effects. Then if, say, a preamp appears to improve detail,
it could be a simple and mild HF boost. Because this also increases any edginess
in the source material, a simple treble tone control could make this preamp's
use satisfactory. This sounds obvious, but it serves as an example of the potentially
over whelming number of variables affecting overall sound quality.
POSSIBLE EFFECTS OF THE SWITCH AND ATTENUATOR
1. I mention this in the interest of true science. Remember, "science" doesn't
mean just measurements. The term literally means "the open-minded search
for truth." I personally don't believe the switch and attenuator (if of
good quality) have any perceivable effect, but my mind is open to the possibility.
2. The music signal has already traveled through many resistors, capacitors,
attenuators, cables, amplifying de vices, and possibly transformers (and switches,
connectors, and so on). And the recording medium most likely has far more degradation
than all of the above components used in recording.
3. If you still think the switch and attenuator could have effects, you can
perform an intrinsic fidelity test on them alone (e.g., use a second switch
for the “component under test” in Fig. 1). The same applies to cables and connectors.
Note: I use Caig “Deoxit Gold G5” (formerly called “Pro Gold”) spray on all
connections. Using this on clean tin-plated connector provides a better contact
than a dirty gold-plated one!
RESULTS OF SOME TESTS
First, a note on my hearing. For the scientists, my threshold is within 3dB
of the standard “good hearing for young people” audiogram. I have a good friend
whose threshold is 20dB above (less sensitive than) the standard audiogram,
yet he can easily hear the superior resolution of vinyl and SACDs (in that
order) over 16/44 CDs. For the audiophile, my hearing has been praised by Ed
Dell and Joe D’Appolito.
I’ve performed the intrinsic fidelity test on two components so far:
1. “Mad Katy” 250W stereo tube amp: There were infrequent times when I thought
I might have heard a difference, but if so it was on the edge of my perceptibility
Furthermore, very extensive listening made me doubt there was any audible difference.
2. “The LP797 Ultra-Low Distortion Phono Preamp, preceded by a stereo pair
of very ac curate (±0.03dB) inverse RIAA net works (“Tweaking the Passive In
verse RIAA Network,”), overall (combined) gain trimmed to ±0.01dB at 1kHz.
I heard absolutely zero difference. No coloration, distortion, noise, hum,
nor loss of spatial or tonal resolution, nor change in dynamics.
3. While not an intrinsic fidelity test, I performed a conventional A/B comparison
of the outputs of “Mad Katy” and a 70W per channel solid-state amp. Using both
recorded music and live miked voice and a bell, I easily heard the difference.
The tube amp had that natural voice “roundness of tone,” and very natural reproduction
of the bell’s transient “clink” and resonating “clang.” (I also compared the
reproduced sound on excellent ribbon-tweeter speakers, to the live bell and
voice). The solid-state amp, by comparison, made vowel sounds slightly “constricted,”
and made the bell sound slightly “electronic,” in stead of like real metal
vibrating.
COMMENTS
The following are my personal opinions, based on much observation—but nevertheless
they are beliefs, and as such are open to debate. I welcome your (favor able
or unfavorable) responses. All I ask is that they are respectful; please don’t
accuse me of being biased or having tin ears!
1. In the phono preamp test, with no audible difference, the signal traveled
through eight caps (two in the in verse RIAA network), many resistors, one
JFET, three op amps, and some ordinary (but good quality and clean) RCA cables.
All resistors were 1% metal film (but not “exotic” brands). All caps were
metalized polypropylene. All connections were cleaned and sprayed with G5.
The unity-gain trimpots (in the inverse RIAA network) were precision 14-turn
cermet units.
Because of the no-sonic-difference result (very carefully auditioned for an
hour of switching), I conclude that claims of the audibility of (ordinary but
good quality) resistors, film caps, wire, and the best op amps are un founded.
2. I believe that such claims are the result of one or more of these factors:
a) The claimants’ assertions are based on memory (listening, soldering in
a different resistor, and soon, then after many minutes listening again). Memory
is notoriously unreliable (ask any courtroom judge), plus you’re probably not re-listening in exactly the same position in the room; this strongly influences
the frequency response you hear. Also, the SPL is probably not matched to the
required 0.2dB or better.
b) The influence of beliefs and expectations. Please don’t be insulted; these
influences are well known by audiological researchers. There was a story in
Stereophile of an experienced audiophile who bought a simple wall clock that
the manufacturer claimed (if you can believe this) would “purify the AC line
from timing imperfections in the whole house!” (and I’ll sell you the Golden
Gate Bridge for $10). It was the infamous “Tice Clock.”
Anyway, the audiophile installed this clock, behind his refrigerator out of
sight. After about a month, he told a friend that the clock improved the sound
of his very expensive music system—better resolution, less noise, and so on.
Then his friend revealed the truth to him: on the day the audiophile installed
the clock, the friend secretly unplugged it! The poor audiophile wouldn’t speak
to his friend for several months!
c) Here’s a factor that’s less controversial: Many circuit designs can be
overly sensitive to a component’s value and its parasitics (e.g., the Effective
Series Resistance of a cap). This can be caused by potentially unstable feedback
loops, power supply interactions, excessive tube or transistor bias-point changes
(with signal amplitude, line voltage,’ or temperature), RF interference, and
so on. Such factors can also make a component overly sensitive to cable capacitance
and inductance. You can merely replace a 5” piece of wire, then notice a difference.
But the heat of soldering could permanently change a resistor’s value or the
leakage of a cap, not to mention the surface leakage of a PC board that had
been high due to humidity. If the circuit is overly sensitive to such things,
the audible difference would be wrongly attributed to the atomic structure
of the replaced wire! (Another factor could be different ground capacitance
if the new wire position is different).
3. If you really believe that, say, a 5” piece of wire can make an audible
difference, please perform a careful intrinsic fidelity test on it. If you
then hear a consistently reliable difference, I’ll believe you and not use
that kind of wire!
4. I realize that it’s possible to have (as an example) ten elements (resistors,
wire, and so on) that individually have a degradation that’s below audibility,
but when combined these degradations (especially if of the same nature and
polarity) sum to an audible level. This is, of course, a good reason’ to use
the highest affordable quality parts, at least up to a point.
5. The opposite side of that argument is the extremely revealing nature of
a very careful and extensive intrinsic fidelity test. If the differences heard
are very small, the component’s effect will likely not be audible in normal
use.
6. There’s some opinion that after listening normally for some time—say several
months— you can notice subtle effects that aren’t revealed in, say, an hour
of even rigorous evaluation. I find this to be untrue; I’ve listened with some
particular components for over a year, and any “accumulated observations” were
revealed in that first hour of intrinsic fidelity testing, and with greater
sensitivity Furthermore, hearing has evolved to be very adaptable in “tuning
out” benign anomalies such as small amounts of 2nd harmonic distortion and
mild frequency response deviations.
CONCLUSION
Intrinsic fidelity testing serves two purposes: It allows the most sensitive
evaluation of an electronic component, free of other (system and source) variables,
and it can be very informative about the effects of various circuits, topologies,
and parts, on sound quality.
|